UMR CNRS 5023

Laboratoire d'Ecologie des Hydrosystèmes Naturels et Anthropisés


  • 001
  • 002
  • 003
  • 004
  • 005
  • 006
  • 007
  • 008
  • 009
  • 010
  • 011
  • 012
  • 013
  • 014
  • 015
  • 016
  • 017
  • 018
  • 019
  • 020
  • 021
  • 022
  • 023
  • 024
logo LEHNA

UMR CNRS 5023

Laboratoire d'Ecologie des Hydrosystèmes
Naturels et Anthropisés

DUTOUR Mylène
ATER

Post-doctorant : E2C

Université Lyon 1
CNRS, UMR 5023 - LEHNA,
Laboratoire d'Ecologie des Hydrosystèmes Naturels et Anthropisés
3, rue Raphaël Dubois - Bât. Darwin C
F-69622 Villeurbanne Cedex FRANCE

Cette adresse e-mail est protégée contre les robots spammeurs. Vous devez activer le JavaScript pour la visualiser.






  • Mes travaux de recherche portent sur la communication animale, les mécanismes cognitifs qui la sous-tendent, ainsi que leur évolution. Je m’intéresse particulièrement à la communication acoustique entre espèces, car ce type de communication soulève de nouvelles questions et défis dans le domaine de la communication animale. J'ai initialement étudié ces interactions entre différentes espèces de passereaux sauvages en France, avant de poursuivre mes recherches sur les interactions interspécifiques chez les oiseaux en Australie, puis sur la communication entre les humains et les animaux. Un autre aspect de mes travaux consiste à étudier comment les oiseaux utilisent la syntaxe – une caractéristique du langage humain selon laquelle le sens d'une séquence dépend des unités individuelles et de l'ordre dans lequel elles sont combinées – pour coder l'information dans leurs cris. Mon projet de recherche en tant qu'ATER se concentre sur la communication visuelle et acoustique chez les mésanges, ce qui pourrait offrir de nouvelles perspectives sur les théories de l'évolution du langage.

    My research focuses on animal communication, the cognitive mechanisms that underlie it, and its evolution. I am particularly interested in acoustic communication between species, as this type of communication raises new questions and challenges in the field of animal communication. I initially studied these interactions between different species of wild songbirds in France, before continuing my research on interspecies interactions in birds in Australia, and then exploring human-animal communication. Another aspect of my work involves studying how birds use syntax—a characteristic of human language in which the meaning of a sequence depends on the individual units and the order in which they are combined—to encode information in their calls. My current research project focuses on visual and acoustic communication in tits, which could provide new insights into the theories of language evolution.

  • Publications issues des travaux de post-doctorat

    29. Randler C, & Dutour M. (2024). Birding specialization and satisfaction in Australian birders – a Big Year is not a big issue. World Leisure Journal. [IF=1.93]

    28. Ferreira VHB, […https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0168159123003106">Applied Animal Behaviour Science. [IF=2.57https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/ibi.13224">Ibis. [IF=2.35]

    27. Dutour M, & Cordonnier M. (2024). The number of Great Tit mobbers influences the mobbing response of heterospecific birds. Ibis. [IF=2.35https://brill.com/view/journals/beh/161/1/article-p89_6.xml">Behaviour. [IF=1.40]

    26. Randler C, Kalb N, & Dutour M. (2023). Nuthatch (Sitta europaea) responses towards simulated territorial intrusion (STI) vary in strength during the non-breeding season - a matter of temperature and photoperiod? Behaviour. [IF=1.40]

    25. Cordonnier M, Ridley M, Lengagne T, & Dutour M. (2023). The impact of high temperatures on birds responses to alarm calls. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. [IF=2.94]

    24. Fernández GJ, Dutour M, & Carro ME. (2023). Information transfer during mobbing: call rate is more important than the number of callers in a southern temperate passerine. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. [IF=2.94]

    23. Dutour M, Walsh SL, Blackburn G, & Ridley AR. (2023). Western Australian magpies respond to urgency information contained in conspecific alarm calls. Behaviour. [IF=1.67]

    22. Blackburn G, Ridley AR, & Dutour M. (2023). Australian magpies discriminate between the territorial calls of intra and extra-group conspecifics. Ibis. [IF=2.35]

    21. Ferreira VHB, Dutour M, Oscarsson R, Gjøen J, & Jensen P. (2022). Effects of domestication on responses of chickens and red junglefowl to conspecific calls: A pilot study. PLoS One. [IF=3.75]

    20. Dutour M, Fernández GJ, & Randler C. (2022). How great tits respond to urgency-based information in allopatric Southern house wren mobbing calls. Ethology. [IF=1.89]

    19. Dutour M, Ridley AR, & Randler C. (2022). Is the urgency message encoded in heterospecific calls perceived by domestic chickens? Journal of Zoology. [IF=2.32]

    18. Dutour M. (2022). Season does not influence the response of great tits to allopatric mobbing calls. Journal of Ethology. [IF=1.20]

    17. Dutour M, Kasper J, & Ridley AR. (2021). Transfer of information between a highly social species and heterospecific community members. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

    16. Dutour M, Walsh SL, Speechley EM, & Ridley AR. (2021). Female Australian magpies discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar human voices. Ethology. [IF=1.76]

    15. Dutour M, & Randler C. (2021). Mobbing responses of great tits (Parus major) do not depend on the number of heterospecific callers. Ethology. [IF=1.76]

    14. Dutour M, Kalb N, Salis A, & Randler C. (2021). Number of callers may affect the response to conspecific mobbing calls in great tits (Parus major). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. [IF=2.94]

    13. Dutour M, & Danel S. (2021). Wild great tits’ alarm calls prompt vigilant behaviours in free-range chickens. Animal Cognition. [IF=2.93]

    12. Dutour M, Walsh SL, & Ridley AR. (2021). Australian magpies adjust their alarm calls according to predator distance. Bioacoustics. [IF=2.01]

    11. Dutour M, Suzuki T, & Wheatcroft D. (2020). Great tit responses to the calls of an unfamiliar species suggest conserved perception of call ordering. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. [IF=2.98]

    10. Dutour M, & Ridley AR. (2020). Females sing more often and at higher frequencies than males in Australian magpies. Behavioural Processes. [IF=1.78]
    Souligné dans ABC News (Perth, Australie)

    9. Dutour M, Lévy L, Lengagne T, Holveck MJ, Crochet PA, Perret P, Doutrelant C, & Grégoire A. (2020). Hissing like a snake: bird hisses are similar to snake hisses and prompt similar anxiety behavior in a mammalian model. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. [IF=2.98]
    Souligné dans Psychology Today: Birds hiss like snakes to protect nests. Mary Bates.


    Publications issues des travaux de thèse

    8. Dutour M, Lengagne T, & Léna JP. (2024). Relative divergence of mobbing calls and songs structures in passerine birds. Journal of Ornithology. [IF=2.94]

    7. Salis A, Léna JP, Lengagne T, & Dutour M. (2021). Biological conclusions on importance of order in mobbing calls vary with the reproductive context in Great tits. Ibis. [IF=2.35]

    6. Dutour M, Léna JP, Dumet A, Gardette V, Mondy N, & Lengagne T. (2019). The role of associative learning process on the response of fledgling great tits (Parus major) to mobbing calls. Animal Cognition. [IF=2.58]

    5. Dutour M, Lengagne T, & Léna JP. (2019). Syntax manipulation changes perception of mobbing call sequences across passerine species. Ethology. [IF=1.70]

    4. Dutour M, Cordonnier M, Léna JP, & Lengagne T. (2019). Seasonal variation in mobbing behaviour of passerine birds. Journal of Ornithology. [IF=1.29]

    3. Dutour M, Léna JP, & Lengagne T. (2017). Mobbing calls: a signal transcending species boundaries. Animal Behaviour. [IF=3.07]

    2. Dutour M, Léna JP, & Lengagne T. (2017). Mobbing behaviour in a passerine community increases with prevalence in predator diet. Ibis. [IF=2.23]

    1. Dutour M, Léna JP, & Lengagne T. (2016). Mobbing behaviour varies according to predator dangerousness and occurrence. Animal Behaviour. [IF=2.87]
    Souligné dans Ornithos, revue des ornithologues de terrain.






     




Site de la Doua
Université Claude Bernard - Lyon I
CNRS, UMR 5023 - LEHNA (Laboratoire d'Ecologie des Hydrosystèmes Naturels et Anthropisés)
3-6, rue Raphaël Dubois - Bâtiments Darwin C & Forel, 69622 Villeurbanne Cedex
43, Boulevard du 11 novembre 1918
Plan d'accès
Tél. : (33) 4 72 43 29 53 - Fax : (33) 4 72 43 11 41
Site de Vaulx-en-Velin
ENTPE
CNRS, UMR 5023 - LEHNA (Laboratoire d'Ecologie des Hydrosystèmes Naturels et Anthropisés)
3, rue Maurice Audin
69518 Vaulx-en-Velin
Plan d'accès
Tél : (33) 04 72 04 70 56 - Fax : (33) 04 72 04 77 43